Skip to main content
School of Languages, Linguistics and Film

LingLunch | Linnaea Stockall (QMUL)

23 October 2019

Time: 1:00 - 2:00pm
Venue: Grad Center 104

Our very own Dr. Linnaea Stockall will give a talk on

 

Systematicity and Variation in Word Structure Processing Across Languages: a Neuro-Typology approach (SAVaNT)

 

She will use the linglunch to crowd-source some feedback on the grant proposal she is preparing to submit to the ESRC in a few weeks. She will present the narrative as it’s structured in the Case for Support, and also present the Pathways to Impact programme. The project needs to sound convincing to both expert reviewers and to assessment Panel B, which covers Education, Linguistics, Social Work, Science and Technology Studies, Socio-Legal Studies and Sociology. She hopes a broad range of colleagues and students will come along to offer their feedback. The presentation should also be useful for students interested in the grant writing process.

 

Below is the draft of the max 4000 character SUMMARY FOR GENERAL AUDIENCE:

 

Speakers of languages know many things about their language that they have never been taught, and probably do not even know that they know. For example, speakers of English know that the prefix re- attaches to verbs (eg. refill, repaint). Attaching re- to nouns creates impossible words: reidea, resofa. Speakers of English also know that re- can only attach to specific kinds of verbs like ‘fill’ and ‘open’ that describe an event which causes a change of state (fill = cause something to become filled). If we attach re- to verbs which do not have the right meaning, the result is not a possible word of English: reknow, resmile. Every human language has rules like this which place limits on the way pieces of the language can be combined to make new words. A combination that is impossible in English, like relaugh, is perfectly fine in French (resourir) or Greek (xanagelo).

 

We can test people’s unconscious knowledge of these kinds of restrictions with a very simple experiment, in which we show speakers real English words, and made up, impossible words like reknow, one word at a time, and ask them to say whether the word is a possible word or not. As people are doing this experiment, we use neuroimaging equipment to record their brain activity to find out how their brains process these impossible words. We have found that both English and Greek speakers’ brains produce the same patterns of activity. When speakers of these two languages read a word that combines a prefix or suffix with a stem of the wrong category (eg. reidea), their brains produce more activity in a region in the left temporal lobe about 200ms after they first see the word. But Greek and English speakers produce a different response when they read a word that combines a prefix or suffix with a stem that is the right category, but has the wrong semantics (eg. reknow): in this case, their brains produce more activity about 450ms after they first see the word, and the response comes from the frontal lobe. Just from these two languages, it seems that human brains have the same kinds of responses in the same locations, and at the same times, to similar kinds of linguistic information across different languages. But Greek and English are only two of thousands of languages, and they are both members of the Indo-European language family.

 

In general, research on how the human brain processes language has mostly focused on a very small set of familiar, related European languages like English, Dutch, German, Spanish and French. We know almost nothing about how the brains of speakers of most of the world’s languages respond to even simple linguistic tasks like processing a single word.

 

Our project will test speakers from a wider range of different languages, which have a range of different word formation rules and processes. These languages will include two other Indo-European languages: Slovenian, a Slavic language in which verbs usually have four or five separate pieces (morphemes), to mark things like tense, aspect, and the person, number and gender of the subject; and Bangla, an Indo-Aryan language in which verbs often change their pronunciation to mark grammatical features (like English sing~sang or write~written). We will also include Arabic, a Semitic language, in which words are made by combining a 3 consonant root like KTB with different vowel ‘melodies’ (eg. kitab = book, kaatib = writer, katab = ‘to read’), and Tagalog, an Austronesian language, in which words can be made by doubling part of the word (eg. ‘halo’ = ‘a mix’, ‘hahalo’ = ‘to mix’), or by inserting an affix into the middle of the word (eg. ‘h-in-alo’ = ‘it was mixed’). We’ll use the same simple experiment to show speakers of these languages words which break either a category rule (reidea) or a semantic rule (unsmile) to see whether the brain responses we observed for English and Greek are truly universal, and how different word-formation processes might use the same basic language network in different ways.

 

For more information, please contact events organiser Elvis Coimbra-Gomes (ling-events@qmul.ac.uk).
Back to top